Accesso libero

The influence of sentential context and frequency of occurrence on the recognition of words with scrambled letters

INFORMAZIONI SU QUESTO ARTICOLO

Cita

Coltheart, M. Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual-route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204-256.10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind: an essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/4737.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Gleason, J. B. & Ratner, N. B. (Ed.) (1998; Polish edition: 2005). Psycholingwistyka. Gdańsk: GWP.Search in Google Scholar

Gough, P. B., Alford, J. A., & Holley-Wilcox, P. (1981). Words and contexts. In O. J. L. Tzeng & H. Singer (Eds.), Perception of print. Reading research in experimental psychology (pp. 85-102). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Grainger, J. (1990). Word Frequency and Neighborhood Frequency Effects in Lexical Decision and Naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 228-244.10.1016/0749-596X(90)90074-ASearch in Google Scholar

Grainger, J. & Whitney, C. (2004) Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe?, Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 58-59.10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.006Search in Google Scholar

Howes, D. H. & Solomon, R. L. (1951). Visual duration threshold as a function of word probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 401-410.10.1037/h0056020Search in Google Scholar

Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., & Quinlan, P. T. (1990). Orthographic processing in visual word identification. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 517-560.10.1016/0010-0285(90)90012-SSearch in Google Scholar

Korpus jezyka polskiego PWN.Search in Google Scholar

McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375-405.10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375Search in Google Scholar

Miller, G. A. (1951) Language and communication. New York: McGraw-Hill.Search in Google Scholar

Simpson, G. B., Peterson, R. R., Casteel, M. A., & Burgess, C. (1989). Lexical and sentence context effects in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 88-97.Search in Google Scholar

Swinney, D. A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 120-136.10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90355-4Search in Google Scholar

Tabossi, P. (1988). Accessing lexical ambiguity in different types of sentential contexts. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 324-340.10.1016/0749-596X(88)90058-7Search in Google Scholar

Whaley, C. P. (1978). Word-nonword classification time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 143-154.10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90110-XSearch in Google Scholar

Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: the SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Buletin and Review, 8, 221-243.10.3758/BF03196158Search in Google Scholar

Velan, H., & Frost, R (2007). Cambridge University versus Hebrew University: The impact of letter transposition on Reading English and Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14 (5), 913-918.10.3758/BF03194121Search in Google Scholar

ISSN:
1234-2238
Lingua:
Inglese
Frequenza di pubblicazione:
Volume Open
Argomenti della rivista:
Social Sciences, Psychology, Applied Psychology