Open Access

The influence of sentential context and frequency of occurrence on the recognition of words with scrambled letters


Cite

Coltheart, M. Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual-route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204-256.10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. (1983). Modularity of mind: an essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/4737.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Gleason, J. B. & Ratner, N. B. (Ed.) (1998; Polish edition: 2005). Psycholingwistyka. Gdańsk: GWP.Search in Google Scholar

Gough, P. B., Alford, J. A., & Holley-Wilcox, P. (1981). Words and contexts. In O. J. L. Tzeng & H. Singer (Eds.), Perception of print. Reading research in experimental psychology (pp. 85-102). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Grainger, J. (1990). Word Frequency and Neighborhood Frequency Effects in Lexical Decision and Naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 228-244.10.1016/0749-596X(90)90074-ASearch in Google Scholar

Grainger, J. & Whitney, C. (2004) Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a wlohe?, Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 58-59.10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.006Search in Google Scholar

Howes, D. H. & Solomon, R. L. (1951). Visual duration threshold as a function of word probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 401-410.10.1037/h0056020Search in Google Scholar

Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., & Quinlan, P. T. (1990). Orthographic processing in visual word identification. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 517-560.10.1016/0010-0285(90)90012-SSearch in Google Scholar

Korpus jezyka polskiego PWN.Search in Google Scholar

McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375-405.10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375Search in Google Scholar

Miller, G. A. (1951) Language and communication. New York: McGraw-Hill.Search in Google Scholar

Simpson, G. B., Peterson, R. R., Casteel, M. A., & Burgess, C. (1989). Lexical and sentence context effects in word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 88-97.Search in Google Scholar

Swinney, D. A. (1979). Lexical access during sentence comprehension: (Re)consideration of context effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 120-136.10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90355-4Search in Google Scholar

Tabossi, P. (1988). Accessing lexical ambiguity in different types of sentential contexts. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 324-340.10.1016/0749-596X(88)90058-7Search in Google Scholar

Whaley, C. P. (1978). Word-nonword classification time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 143-154.10.1016/S0022-5371(78)90110-XSearch in Google Scholar

Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: the SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Buletin and Review, 8, 221-243.10.3758/BF03196158Search in Google Scholar

Velan, H., & Frost, R (2007). Cambridge University versus Hebrew University: The impact of letter transposition on Reading English and Hebrew. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14 (5), 913-918.10.3758/BF03194121Search in Google Scholar

ISSN:
1234-2238
Language:
English
Publication timeframe:
Volume Open
Journal Subjects:
Social Sciences, Psychology, Applied Psychology