Acceso abierto

How Does Ritualized Behavior Lower Anxiety? The Role of Cognitive Load and Conscious Preoccupation in Anxiety Reduction


Cite

Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F. & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis of controlled studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(5), 683–703. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00057-X10.1016/S0272-7358(00)00057-XSearch in Google Scholar

Anastasi, M. W. & Newberg, A. B. (2008). A preliminary study of the acute effects of religious ritual on anxiety. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 14(2), 163–165. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2007.067510.1089/acm.2007.0675Search in Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2012). Working Memory: Theories, Models, and Controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-10042210.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422Search in Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., Gollwitzer, P. M. & Trötschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 1014–1027. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.101410.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1014Search in Google Scholar

Berry, L. M. & Laskey, B. (2012). A review of obsessive intrusive thoughts in the general population. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 1(2), 125–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2012.02.00210.1016/j.jocrd.2012.02.002Search in Google Scholar

Boyer, P. & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595–613. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0600933210.1017/S0140525X06009332Search in Google Scholar

Brooks, A. W., Schroeder, J., Risen, J. L., Gino, F., Galinsky, A. D., Norton, M. I. & Schweitzer, M. E. (2016). Don’t stop believing: Rituals improve performance by decreasing anxiety. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 137(1), 71–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.07.00410.1016/j.obhdp.2016.07.004Search in Google Scholar

Clark, D. A. (Ed.). (2005). Intrusive thoughts in clinical disorders: Theory, research, and treatment. Guilford Press.Search in Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-14375010.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750Search in Google Scholar

Doallo, S., Holguín, S. R. & Cadaveira, F. (2006). Attentional load affects automatic emotional processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. NeuroReport, 17(17), 1797–1801. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000246325.51191.3910.1097/01.wnr.0000246325.51191.39Search in Google Scholar

Dvorak-Bertsch, J. D., Curtin, J. J., Rubinstein, T. J. & Newman, J. P. (2007). Anxiety moderates the interplay between cognitive and affective processing. Psychological Science, 18(8), 699–705. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01963.x10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01963.xSearch in Google Scholar

Eitam, B. & Higgins, E. T. (2010). Motivation in mental accessibility: Relevance of a representation (ROAR) as a new framework. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(10), 951–967. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00309.x10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00309.xSearch in Google Scholar

Eitam, B. & Higgins, E. T. (2016). From reaction (‘priming’) to motivated selection: Changing conceptualizations of accessibility. Current Opinion in Psychology, 12, 58–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.01710.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.017Search in Google Scholar

Erber, R. & Tesser, A. (1992). Task effort and the regulation of mood: The absorption hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28(4), 339–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(92)90050-T10.1016/0022-1031(92)90050-TSearch in Google Scholar

Erk, S., Abler, B. & Walter, H. (2006). Cognitive modulation of emotion anticipation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24(4), 1227–1236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04976.x10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04976.xSearch in Google Scholar

Erk, S., Kleczar, A. & Walter, H. (2007). Valence-specific regulation effects in a working memory task with emotional context. NeuroImage, 37(2), 623–632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.00610.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.006Search in Google Scholar

Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: a dual-process framework. Cognition and Emotion, 25(3), 400–412. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.54416010.1080/02699931.2010.544160Search in Google Scholar

Hobson, N. M., Schroeder, J., Risen, J. L., Xygalatas, D. & Inzlicht, M. (2018). The psychology of rituals: An integrative review and process-based framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(3), 260–284. https://doi.org/10.1177/108886831773494410.1177/1088868317734944Search in Google Scholar

Ju, Y. J. & Lien, Y. W. (2016). Better control with less effort: The advantage of using focused-breathing strategy over focused-distraction strategy on thought suppression, Consciousness and Cognition, 40(1), 9–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.00210.1016/j.concog.2015.12.002Search in Google Scholar

Julien, D., O’Connor, K. P. & Aardema, F. (2009). Intrusions related to obsessive-compulsive disorder: a question of content or context? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(7), 709–722. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.2057810.1002/jclp.20578Search in Google Scholar

Karl, J. A. & Fischer, R. (2018). Rituals, repetitiveness and cognitive load. Human Nature, 29(1), 418–441. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9325-310.1007/s12110-018-9325-3Search in Google Scholar

King, R. & Schaefer, A. (2011). The emotional startle effect is disrupted by a concurrent working memory task. Psychophysiology, 48(2), 269–272. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01062.x10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01062.xSearch in Google Scholar

Krátký, J., Lang, M., Shaver, J. H., Jerotijević, D. & Xygalatas, D. (2016). Anxiety and ritualization: Can attention discriminate compulsion from routine? Communicative and Integrative Biology, 9(3). https://doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2016.117479910.1080/19420889.2016.1174799Search in Google Scholar

Lang, M., Krátký, J., Shaver, J. H., Jerotijević, D. & Xygalatas, D. (2015). Effects of anxiety on spontaneous ritualized behavior. Current Biology, 25(14), 1892–1897. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.04910.1016/j.cub.2015.05.049Search in Google Scholar

Lim, S. L., Padmala, S. & Pessoa, L. (2008). Affective learning modulates spatial competition during low-load attentional conditions. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1267–1278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.00310.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.003Search in Google Scholar

Lin, Y. J. & Wicker, F. W. (2007). A comparison of the effects of thought suppression, distraction and concentration. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(12), 2924–2937. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.08.00410.1016/j.brat.2007.08.004Search in Google Scholar

Luciano, J. V. & González, S. A. (2007). Analysis of the efficacy of different thought suppression strategies. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 7(3), 335–345.Search in Google Scholar

MacNamara, A., Ferri, J. & Hajcak, G. (2011). Working memory load reduces the late positive potential and this effect is attenuated with increasing anxiety. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 11(3), 321–331. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-011-0036-z10.3758/s13415-011-0036-zSearch in Google Scholar

Magee, J. C., Dreyer-Oren, S. E., Sarfan, L. D., Teachman, B. A. & Clerkin, E. M. (2019). Don’t tell me what to think: Comparing self- and other-generated distraction methods for controlling intrusive thinking. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.12.00310.1016/j.jocrd.2017.12.003Search in Google Scholar

Magee, J. C., Harden, K. P. & Teachman, B. A. (2012). Psychopathology and thought suppression: A quantitative review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32(3), 189–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.00110.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.001Search in Google Scholar

Malinowski, B. (1948). Magic, Science, and Religion. In B. Malinowski, Magic, Science and Religion, and Other Essays (pp. 1–71). Boston: Beacon Press.Search in Google Scholar

May, J., Andrade, J., Batey, H., Berry, L. M. & Kavanagh, D. J. (2010). Less food for thought. Impact of attentional instructions on intrusive thoughts about snack foods. Appetite, 55(2), 279–287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.06.01410.1016/j.appet.2010.06.014Search in Google Scholar

Mitchell, D. G. V., Nakic, M., Fridberg, D., Kamel, N., Pine, D. S. & Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The impact of processing load on emotion. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1299–1309. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.01210.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.012Search in Google Scholar

Moors, A. & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 297–326. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.29710.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297Search in Google Scholar

Moran, T. P. (2016). Anxiety and working memory capacity: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142(8), 831–864. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul000005110.1037/bul0000051Search in Google Scholar

Najmi, S., Riemann, B. C. & Wegner, D. M. (2009). Managing unwanted intrusive thoughts in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Relative effectiveness of suppression, focused distraction, and acceptance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(6), 494–503. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.02.01510.1016/j.brat.2009.02.015Search in Google Scholar

Pennycook, G. (2018). A perspective on the theoretical foundation of dual-process models. In W. De Neys (Ed.), Dual Process Theory 2.0 (pp. 5–27). New York: Psychology Press.Search in Google Scholar

Pessoa, L., Padmala, S. & Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. NeuroImage, 28(1), 249–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.04810.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.048Search in Google Scholar

Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 23(5), 571–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-610.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6Search in Google Scholar

Salkovskis, P. M. & Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 193–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)90112-010.1016/0005-7967(94)90112-0Search in Google Scholar

Shackman, A. J., Sarinopoulos, I., Maxwell, J. S., Pizzagalli, D. A., Lavric, A. & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Anxiety selectively disrupts visuospatial working memory. Emotion, 6(1), 40–61. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.4010.1037/1528-3542.6.1.40Search in Google Scholar

Simpson, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Gusnard, D. A. & Raichle, M. E. (2001). Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex: I. During cognitive task performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 683–687. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.98.2.68310.1073/pnas.98.2.683Search in Google Scholar

Slepian, M. L., Oikawa, M. & Smyth, J. M. (2014). Suppressing thoughts of evaluation while being evaluated. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(1), 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.1219710.1111/jasp.12197Search in Google Scholar

Sosis, R. & Handwerker, W. P. (2011). Psalms and coping with uncertainty: Religious Israeli women’s responses to the 2006 Lebanon War. American Anthropologist, 113(1), 40–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1433.2010.01305.x10.1111/j.1548-1433.2010.01305.xSearch in Google Scholar

Spangler, D. P. & Friedman, B. H. (2017). A little goes a long way: Low working memory load is associated with optimal distractor inhibition and increased vagal control under anxiety. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11:43. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.0004310.3389/fnhum.2017.00043Search in Google Scholar

Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I. & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Memory & Cognition, 23(5), 551–559. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF0319725710.3758/BF03197257Search in Google Scholar

Teasdale, J. D., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A. & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and stimulus-independent thought: Effects of memory load and presentation rate. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5(4), 417–433. https://doi.org/10.1080/0954144930852012810.1080/09541449308520128Search in Google Scholar

Tonna, M., Marchesi, C. & Parmigiani, S. (2019). The biological origins of rituals: An interdisciplinary perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 98(1), 95–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.03110.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.031Search in Google Scholar

Van Dillen, L. F. & Derks, B. (2012). Working memory load reduces facilitated processing of threatening faces: An ERP study. Emotion, 12(6), 1340–1349. https://doi.org/10.1037/a002862410.1037/a0028624Search in Google Scholar

Van Dillen, L. F., Heslenfeld, D. J. & Koole, S. L. (2009). Tuning down the emotional brain: An fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images.NeuroImage, 45(4), 1212–1219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.01610.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.016Search in Google Scholar

Van Dillen, L. F. & Koole, S. L. (2007). Clearing the mind: A working memory model of distraction from negative mood. Emotion, 7(4), 715–723. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.71510.1037/1528-3542.7.4.715Search in Google Scholar

Van Dillen, L. F. & Koole, S. L. (2009). How automatic is “automatic vigilance”? The role of working memory in attentional interference of negative information. Cognition and Emotion, 23(6), 1106–1117. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269993080233817810.1080/02699930802338178Search in Google Scholar

Vytal, K., Cornwell, B., Arkin, N. & Grillon, C. (2012). Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: From impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load. Psychophysiology, 49(6), 842–852. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01358.x10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01358.xSearch in Google Scholar

Wang, D., Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2020). Ironic Effects of Thought Suppression: A Meta-Analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(3), 778–793. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/c64fp10.31234/osf.io/c64fpSearch in Google Scholar

Wang, D., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. & Hagger, M. S. (2017). Mechanisms underlying effective thought suppression using focused-distraction strategies: A self-determination theory approach. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory Research, and Practice, 4(4), 367–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns000013810.1037/cns0000138Search in Google Scholar

Wang, D., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. & Hagger, M. S. (2018). Why distractors with need-supportive content can mitigate ironic effects of thought suppression. Motivation and Emotion, 42(2), 214–224. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-017-9653-310.1007/s11031-017-9653-3Search in Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101(1), 34–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.101.1.3410.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34Search in Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M. (2009). How to think, say, or do precisely the worst thing for any occasion. Science, 325(5936), 48–50. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.116734610.1126/science.1167346Search in Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M. & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 903–912. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.90310.1037/0022-3514.63.6.903Search in Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M., Erber, R. & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1093–1104. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.109310.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1093Search in Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R. & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.510.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5Search in Google Scholar

Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Knutson, B. & McMahon, S. R. (1991). Polluting the stream of consciousness: The effect of thought suppression on the mind’s environment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15(2), 141–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF0117320410.1007/BF01173204Search in Google Scholar

Wenzlaff, R. M. & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 59–91. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.5910.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.59Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
2199-6059
ISSN:
0860-150X
Idioma:
Inglés
Calendario de la edición:
4 veces al año
Temas de la revista:
Philosophy, other