Open Access

Metacognition and Multiple Strategies in a Cognitive Model of Online Control

   | Nov 23, 2011
Journal of Artificial General Intelligence's Cover Image
Journal of Artificial General Intelligence
Cognitive Architectures, Model Comparison, and AGI, Editors: Christian Lebiere, Cleotilde Gonzalez and Walter Warwick

Cite

Anderson, J. R.; Bothell, D.; Byrne, M. D.; Douglass, S.; Lebiere, C.; and Quin, Y. 2004. An integrated theory of mind. Psychological Review 111:1036-1060.10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.1036Search in Google Scholar

Anderson, J. R. 1991. The place of cognitive architectures in a rational analysis. Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1-24.Search in Google Scholar

Anderson, J. R. 1996. Implicit Memory and Metacognition: Why is the glass half full? In Reder, L. M., ed., Implicit Memory and Cognition. Hillsdale (NJ): Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 123-136.Search in Google Scholar

Ball, J. T.; Gluck, K. A.; Kursmark, M. A.; and Rodgers, S. M. 2003. Comparing three variants of a computational process model of basic aircraft maneuvering. In Proceedings of the 12th Conference on Behavior Representations in Modeling and Simulations, 87-98.Search in Google Scholar

Berry, D. C., and Broadbent, D. E. 1987. The combination of explicit and implicit learning processes in task control. Psychological Research 49:7-15.10.1007/BF00309197Search in Google Scholar

Bott, L., and Heit, E. 2004. Nonmonotonic extrapolation in function learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 30:38-50.Search in Google Scholar

Campbell, J. D. 2008. Addition by Subtraction. Memory and Cognition 36:1094-1102.10.3758/MC.36.6.1094Search in Google Scholar

Fink, E.; Kaplowitz, S.; and Hubbard, S. M. 2002. Oscillation in Beliefs and Decisions. In The persuasion handbook: developments in theory and practice. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.10.4135/9781412976046.n2Search in Google Scholar

Gluck, K.; Halbruegge, M.; Moore, R.; Reitter, D.; and Stanley, C. 2010. Parameter space exploration in cognitive models. Journal of Artificial General Intelligence 2(2).10.2478/v10229-011-0011-7Search in Google Scholar

Gonzalez, C., and Lebiere, C. 2005. Instance-based cognitive models of decision making. In Zizzo, D., and Courakis, A., eds., Transfer of knowledge in economic decision making. New York: Palgrave McMillan.Search in Google Scholar

Gonzalez, C.; Lerch, F.; and Lebiere, C. 2003. Instance-based learning in dynamic decision making. Cognitive Science 27(4):591-635.10.1207/s15516709cog2704_2Search in Google Scholar

Halbrügge, M. 2010. Keep it simple-A case study of model development in the context of the Dynamic Stocks and Flows (DSF) task. Journal of Artificial General Intelligence (this issue).10.2478/v10229-011-0008-2Search in Google Scholar

Jones, R. M.; Laird, J. E.; Nielsen, P. E.; Coulter, K. J.; Kenny, P.; and Koss, F. V. 1999. Automated intelligent pilots for combat flight simulation. AI Magazine 20:27-42.Search in Google Scholar

Lebiere, C.; Gonzalez, C.; and Warwick, W. 2009. A comparative approach to understanding general intelligence: Predicting cognitive performance in an open-ended dynamic task. In Proceedings of the Second Conference on Artificial General Intelligence, 103-107. Amsterdam/Paris: Atlantis Press.10.2991/agi.2009.2Search in Google Scholar

Lebiere, C. 1999. The dynamics of cognition: An ACT-R model of cognitive arithmetic. Kognitionswissenschaft 8(1):5-19.10.1007/s001970050071Search in Google Scholar

Mané, A. M., and Donchin, E. 1989. The Space Fortress game. Acta Psychologica 71(1-3):17-22.10.1016/0001-6918(89)90003-6Search in Google Scholar

McCloskey, M., and Lindemann, M. 1992. MATHNET: preliminary results from a distributed model of arithmetic fact retrieval. In Campbell., ed., The Nature and Origin of Mathematical Skills. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier.10.1037/e665402011-284Search in Google Scholar

Reder, L. M., and Schunn, C. D. 1996. Metacognition does not imply awareness: Strategy choice is governed by implicit learning and memory. In Reder, L. M., ed., Implicit Memory and Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 45-78.Search in Google Scholar

Reitter, D., and Lebiere, C. 2010. Accountable Modeling in ACT-UP, a Scalable, Rapid-Prototyping ACT-R Implementation. In Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Cognitive Modeling (ICCM), 199-204. Philadelphia, PA.Search in Google Scholar

Reitter, D.; Juvina, I.; Stocco, A.; and Lebiere, C. 2010. Resistance is Futile: Winning Lemonade Market Share through Metacognitive Reasoning in a Three-Agent Cooperative Game. In Proceedings of the 19th Behavior Representation in Modeling & Simulation (BRIMS). Charleston, SC.Search in Google Scholar

Salvucci, D. D. 2006. Modeling driver behavior in a cognitive architecture. Human Factors 48(2):362-380.10.1518/00187200677772441716884055Search in Google Scholar

Taatgen, N., and Lee, F. J. 2003. Production Compilation: A Simple Mechanism to Model Complex Skill Acquisition. Human Factors 45(1):61-76.10.1518/hfes.45.1.61.2722412916582Search in Google Scholar

Vicuso, S. R.; Anderson, J. A.; and Spoehr, K. T. 1989. Representing simple arithmetic in neural networks. In Tiberghien, G., ed., Advanced Cognitive Science: Theory and Applications. Chichester, England: Horwood. 144-164.Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
1946-0163
Language:
English
Publication timeframe:
2 times per year
Journal Subjects:
Computer Sciences, Artificial Intelligence