Open Access

Social Structure and Language Structure: the New Nomothetic Approach

Psychology of Language and Communication's Cover Image
Psychology of Language and Communication
Language as a Tool for Interaction, Issue Editor: Joanna Rączaszek-Leonardi

Cite

Atkinson, Q. (2011a). Linking spatial patterns of language variation to ancient demography and population migrations. Linguistic Typology, 15 (2), 321-332.10.1515/lity.2011.022Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Q.D. (2011b). Phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. Science, 332 (6027), 346-349.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Q. (2012). Response to comments on phonemic diversity supports a serial founder effect model of language expansion from Africa. Science, 335 (6069), 657-657.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Q.D., Meade, A., Venditti, C., Greenhill, S.J., & Pagel, M. (2008). Languages evolve in punctuational bursts. Science, 319 (5863), 588.10.1126/science.1149683Search in Google Scholar

Backhaus, J. & Junghanns, K. (2006). Daytime naps improve procedural motor memory. Sleep Medicine, 7, 508-512.10.1016/j.sleep.2006.04.002Search in Google Scholar

Beckner, C., Blythe, R., Bybee, J., Christiansen, M.H., Croft, W., Ellis, N.C., Holland, J., Ke, J., Larsen-Freeman, D., & Schoenemann, T. (2009). Language is a complex adaptive system: Position paper. Language Learning, 59, 1-26.10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00533.xSearch in Google Scholar

Bialystok, E. & Miller, B. (1999). The problems of age in second-language acquisi­tion: Influences from language, structure, and task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2 (2), 127-145.10.1017/S1366728999000231Search in Google Scholar

Bickel, B. & Nichols, J. (2008). Inflectional synthesis of the verb. In M.S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (chapter 22). Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.Search in Google Scholar

Birdsong, D. & Molis, M. (2001). On the evidence for maturational constraints in second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44 (2), 235-249.10.1006/jmla.2000.2750Search in Google Scholar

Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Henry Holt.Search in Google Scholar

Boguna, M., Pastor-Satorras, R., Diaz-Guilera, A., & Arenas, A. (2004). Models of social networks based on social distance attachment. Physical Review E, 70 (5), 056122.Search in Google Scholar

Bornstein, M. (1973). Color vision and color naming: A psychophysiological hy­pothesis of cultural difference. Psychological Bulletin, 80 (4), 257-285.10.1037/h0034837Search in Google Scholar

Bresnan, J. (2007). Is syntactic knowledge probabilistic? Experiments with the English dative alternation. In S. Featherston & W. Sternefeld (Eds.), Roots: Lin­guistics in search of its evidential base (pp. 75-96). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Burkett, D. & Griffiths, T. (2010). Iterated learning of multiple languages from multiple teachers. In A. Smith, M. Schouwstra, B. de Boer, & K. Smith (Eds.), Evolution of language: Proceedings of the 8th International Conference EVO- LANG 8 (pp. 58-65). Singapore: World Scientific.Search in Google Scholar

Bybee, J. (2011). How plausible is the hypothesis that population size and dispersal are related to phoneme inventory size? introducing and commenting on a debate. Linguistic Typology, 15 (2), 147-153.10.1515/lity.2011.009Search in Google Scholar

Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (2005). The evolutionary origin of morphology. In M. Taller- man (Ed.), Language origins: Perspectives on evolution (pp. 166-184). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Central Intelligence Agency (2009). Sex Ratio. In The World Factbook 2009. Wash­ington, DC: Central Intelligence Agency.Search in Google Scholar

Chambers, J.K. (1995). Sociolinguistic theory: Linguistic variation and its social significance. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. New York: Praeger.Search in Google Scholar

Christiansen, M.H. & Chater, N. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31 (5), 489-508; discussion 509-58.10.1017/S0140525X08004998Search in Google Scholar

Clahsen, H., Felser, C., Neubauer, K., Sato, M., & Silva, R. (2010). Morphological structure in native and non-native language processing. Language Learning, 60 (1), 21-43.10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00550.xSearch in Google Scholar

Croft, W. (2000). Explaining language change: an evolutionary approach. Harlow, Essex: Longman.Search in Google Scholar

Croft, W. (2010). The origins of grammaticalization in the verbalization of experi­ence. Linguistics, 48 (1), 1-48.Search in Google Scholar

Crop Protection Compendium (2008). Acacia confusa. online, accessed 18-04-2011. <http://www.cabi.org>Search in Google Scholar

Currie, T.E., Greenhill, S.J., Gray, R.D., Hasegawa, T., & Mace, R. (2010). Rise and fall of political complexity in island south-east Asia and the Pacific. Nature, 467 (7317), 801-804.Search in Google Scholar

Cysouw, M., Dediu, D., & Moran, S. (2012). Comment on “Phonemic Diversity Supports a Serial Founder Effect Model of Language Expansion from Africa”. Science, 335 (6069), 657.10.1126/science.1208841Search in Google Scholar

Dahl, O. (2004). The growth and maintenance of linguistic complexity. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.71Search in Google Scholar

de Boer, B. (2000). Self-organization in vowel systems. Journal of Phonetics, 28 (4), 441-465.10.1006/jpho.2000.0125Search in Google Scholar

DeGraff, M. (2001). On the origin of creoles: A cartesian critique of“neo” darwinian linguistics. Linguistic Typology, 5, 213-311.Search in Google Scholar

Diamond, J. & Bellwood, P. (2003). Farmers and their languages: The first expan­sions. Science, 300 (5619), 597-603.Search in Google Scholar

Digiulio, D.V., Seidenberg, M., Oleary, D.S., & Raz, N. (1994). Procedural and de­clarative memory: A developmental study. Brain and Cognition, 25 (1), 79-91.10.1006/brcg.1994.1024Search in Google Scholar

Dryer, M.S. (2011). Order of subject, object and verb. In M.S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (chapter 81). Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.Search in Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group-size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16 (4), 681-694.10.1017/S0140525X00032325Search in Google Scholar

Dunn, M., Greenhill, S.J., Levinson, S.C., & Gray, R.D. (2011). Evolved structure of language shows lineage-specific trends in word-order universals. Nature, 473 (7345), 79-82.Search in Google Scholar

Eubank, L. & Gregg, K. (1999). Critical periods and (second) language acquisition: Divide et impera. In D. Birdsong (Ed.), Second language acquisition and the critical period hypothesis (pp. 65-99). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Fagyal, Z., Escobar, A., Swarup, S., Gasser, L., & Lakkaraju, K. (2010). Centers and peripheries: Network roles in language change. Lingua, 120 (8), 2061-2079.10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.001Search in Google Scholar

Ferdinand, V. & Zuidema, W. (2009). Thomas’ Theorem meets Bayes’ Rule: a model of the iterated learning of language. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceed­ings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society CogSci’09 (pp. 1786-1791). Wheat Ridge, CO: Cognitive Science Society.Search in Google Scholar

Ferreira, F. (2005). Psycholinguistics, formal grammars, and cognitive science. The Linguistic Review, 22, 365-380.10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2-4.365Search in Google Scholar

Fillmore, C.J. (1992). ‘Corpuslinguistics’ or ‘computer-aided armchair linguistics’. In J. Svartvik (Ed.), Directions in corpus linguistics. Proceedings of Nobel Sym­posium 82, Stockholm (pp. 35-60). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Garrod, S. (2010). Can iterated learning explain the emergence of graphical symbols? Interaction Studies, 11, 33-50.10.1075/is.11.1.04garSearch in Google Scholar

Gordon, R. (2005). Ethnologue: Languages of the world (15th edition). Dallas: SIL International.Search in Google Scholar

Gray, R.D., Drummond, A.J., & Greenhill, S.J. (2009). Language phylogenies reveal expansion pulses and pauses in pacific settlement. Science, 323 (5913), 479-483.Search in Google Scholar

Gray, R.D., Greenhill, S.J., & Ross, R.M. (2007). The pleasures and perils of darwin- izing culture (with phylogenies). Biological Theory, 2 (4), 360-375.10.1162/biot.2007.2.4.360Search in Google Scholar

Greenberg, J.H. (1963). Universals of language: report of a conference held at Dobbs Ferry, New York, April 13-15, 1961. Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.Search in Google Scholar

Greenhill, S.J., Currie, T.E., & Gray, R.D. (2009). Does horizontal transmission in­validate cultural phylogenies? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276 (1665), 2299-2306.Search in Google Scholar

Griffiths, T., Christian, B., & Kalish, M. (2010). Using category structures to test iterated learning as a method for identifying inductive biases. Cognitive Sci­ence, 32 (1), 68-107.Search in Google Scholar

Hartshorne, J. & Ullman, M. (2006). Why girls say ‘holded’ more than boys. Devel­opmental Science, 9 (1), 21-32.10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00459.xSearch in Google Scholar

Haspelmath, M., Dryer, M.S., Gil, D., & Comrie, B. (2008). World Atlas of Language Structures, volume Available online at <http://wals.info/feature/22>. Accessed on 2011-04-18. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.Search in Google Scholar

Hawks, J. (2008). From genes to numbers: effective population sizes in human evolution. In J.-P. Bocquet-Appel (Ed.), Recent advances in palaeodemography: Data, techniques, patterns (pp. 9-30). Dordrecht: Springer.Search in Google Scholar

Hay, J. & Bauer, L. (2007). Phoneme inventory size and population size. Language, 2, 388-400.10.1353/lan.2007.0071Search in Google Scholar

Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: The tasmanian case. American An­tiquity, 69 (2), 197-214.10.2307/4128416Search in Google Scholar

Hunley, K., Bowern, C., Healy, M., Hunley, K., Bowern, C., & Healy, M. (2012). Re­jection of a serial founder effects model of genetic and linguistic coevolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279 (1736), 2281-2288.Search in Google Scholar

Hurford, J.R. (1991). The evolution of the critical period for language acquisition. Cognition, 40 (3), 159-201.10.1016/0010-0277(91)90024-XSearch in Google Scholar

Hyltenstam, K. & Abrahamsson, N. (2003). Maturational constraints in SLA. In C.J. Doughty & M.H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 539-588). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

International Monetary Fund. Online (2011). World economic outlook, April 2011.Search in Google Scholar

Isbell, L.A. & Young, T. (1996). The evolution of bipedalism in hominids and reduced group size in chimpanzees: Alternative responses to decreasing resource avail­ability. Journal of Human Evolution, 30, 389-397(9).10.1006/jhev.1996.0034Search in Google Scholar

Jaeger, T., Graff, P., Croft, W., & Pontillo, D. (2011). Mixed effect models for genetic and areal dependencies in linguistic typology. Linguistic Typology, 15, 281.10.1515/lity.2011.021Search in Google Scholar

Johnson, J. & Newport, E. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive psychology, 21 (1), 60-99.10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0Search in Google Scholar

Kaestle, F.A. & Smith, D.G. (2001). Ancient mitochondrial DNA evidence for prehistoric population movement: the numic expansion. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 115 (1), 1-12.10.1002/ajpa.1051Search in Google Scholar

Kay, P. & Maffi, L. (2008). Number of basic colour categories. In M. Haspelmath, M.S. Dryer, D. Gil, & B. Comrie (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (chapter 133). Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.Search in Google Scholar

Kay, P. & Regier, T. (2003). Resolving the question of color naming universals. Pro­ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, 9085-9089.10.1073/pnas.1532837100Search in Google Scholar

Kellerman, E. & Sharwood Smith, M. (Eds.) (1986). Crosslinguistic influence in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kirby, S., Cornish, H., & Smith, K. (2008). Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105 (31), 10681-10686.10.1073/pnas.0707835105Search in Google Scholar

Kirby, S., Dowman, M., & Griffiths, T.L. (2007). Innateness and culture in the evolu­tion of language. Proceedings of the National Academy ofSciences of the United States of America, 104 (12), 5241-5245.10.1073/pnas.0608222104Search in Google Scholar

Kirby, S. & Hurford, J. (2002). The emergence of linguistic structure: An overview of the iterated learning model. In A. Cangelosi & D. Parisi (Eds.), Simulating the evolution of language (pp. 121-148). London: Springer.Search in Google Scholar

Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon.Search in Google Scholar

Kuhl, P.K., Andruski, J.E., Chistovich, I.A., Chistovich, L.A., Kozhevnikova, E.V., Ryskina, V.L., Stolyarova, E.I., Sundberg, U., & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277 (5326), 684-686.Search in Google Scholar

Lado, R. (1957). Linguistics across cultures. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Search in Google Scholar

Lee, S. & Hasegawa, T. (2011). Bayesian phylogenetic analysis supports an agricul­tural origin of Japonic languages. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278 (1725), 3662-3669.Search in Google Scholar

Levinson, S. (2006). Introduction: The evolution of culture in a microcosm. In S.C. Levinson & P. Jaisson (Eds.), Evolution and culture: A Fyssen Foundation Sym­posium (pp. 1-41). Cambridge: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Long, M. (1990). Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 12 (3), 251-285.10.1017/S0272263100009165Search in Google Scholar

Lupyan, G. & Dale, R. (2010). Language structure is partly determined by social structure. PLoS ONE, 5 (1), e8559.10.1371/journal.pone.0008559Search in Google Scholar

Lycett, S. & Norton, C. (2010). A demographic model for palaeolithic technological evolution: the case of east asia and the movius line. Quaternary International, 211 (1), 55-65.10.1016/j.quaint.2008.12.001Search in Google Scholar

Lycett, S.J. (2009). Understanding ancient hominin dispersals using artefactual data: a phylogeographic analysis of acheulean handaxes. PLoS One, 4 (10), e7404.10.1371/journal.pone.0007404Search in Google Scholar

Lycett, S.J., Collard, M., & McGrew, W.C. (2009). Cladistic analyses of behavioural variation in wild pan troglodytes: exploring the chimpanzee culture hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 57 (4), 337-349.10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.05.015Search in Google Scholar

MacKeigan, T. & Muth, S.Q. (2006). A grammatical network of Tzotzil Mayan colour terms. In C.P. Biggam & C.J. Kay (Eds.), Progress in Colour Studies. Volume I: Language and culture (pp. 23-36). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Search in Google Scholar

Maddieson, I. (2008). Tone. In M.S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online (chapter 13). Munich: Max Planck Digital Library.Search in Google Scholar

Matisoff, J. (1978). Variational semantics in Tibeto-Burman: The ‘organic’ approach to linguistic comparison. Philadelphia: Institute for the Study of Human Issues.Search in Google Scholar

McPherson, J.M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J.M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homoph- ily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415-444.10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.415Search in Google Scholar

McWhorter, J. (2008). Why Does a Language Undress? Strange Cases in Indonesia. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemaki, & F. Karlsson (Eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change (pp. 167-190). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Search in Google Scholar

Mesoudi, A. & Whiten, A. (2008). The multiple roles of cultural transmission experi­ments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3489-3501.10.1098/rstb.2008.0129Search in Google Scholar

Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K.N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29 (4), 329-47; discussion 347-383.10.1017/S0140525X06009083Search in Google Scholar

Milroy, J. & Milroy, L. (1985). Authority in language. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Search in Google Scholar

Nerbonne, J. (2010). Measuring the diffusion of linguistic change. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365 (1559), 3821-3828.10.1098/rstb.2010.0048Search in Google Scholar

Nettle, D. (1999). Linguistic diversity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Nettle, D. & Dunbar, R.I.M. (1997). Social markers and the evolution of reciprocal exchange. Current Anthropology, 38 (1), 93-99.10.1086/204588Search in Google Scholar

Newman, M.E.J. & Park, J. (2003). Why social networks are different from other types of networks. Physical Review E, 68 (3), 036122.10.1103/PhysRevE.68.036122Search in Google Scholar

Paradis, M. (2004). A neurolinguistic theory of bilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins..10.1075/sibil.18Search in Google Scholar

Pericliev, V. (2004). There is no correlation between the size of a community speak­ing a language and the size of the phonological inventory of that language. Linguistic Typology, 8, 376-383.10.1515/lity.2004.8.3.376Search in Google Scholar

Quillinan, J., Kirby, S., & Smith., K. (2010). Co-evolution of language and social net­work structure through cultural transmission. In A.D. Smith, M. Shouwstra, B. de Boer, & K. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on the Evolution of Language EVOLANG 8 (pp. 475-476). Singapore: World Scientific.Search in Google Scholar

Reali, F. & Griffiths, T. (2009). The evolution of frequency distributions: Relating regularization to inductive biases through iterated learning. Cognition, 111 (3), 317-328.10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.012Search in Google Scholar

Richerson, P.J., Boyd, R., & Bettinger, R.L. (2009). Cultural innovations and demo­graphic change. Human Biology, 81 (2-3), 211-235.10.3378/027.081.0306Search in Google Scholar

Roberts, G. (2010). An experimental study of social selection and frequency of interaction in linguistic diversity. Interaction Studies, 11 (1), 138-159.10.1075/is.11.1.06robSearch in Google Scholar

Roberts, S. & Winters, J. (2012). Constructing knowledge: Nomothetic approaches to language evolution. In L. McCrohon, T. Fujimura, K. Fujita, R. Martin, K. Okanoya, R. Suzuki, & N. Yusa (Eds.), Five approaches to language evolu­tion: Proceedings of the Workshops of the 9th International Conference on the Evolution of Language (pp. 148-157). Singapore: World Scientific.Search in Google Scholar

Sapir, E. (1912). Language and environment. American Anthropologist, 14 (2), 226-242.10.1525/aa.1912.14.2.02a00020Search in Google Scholar

Schachter, J. (1974). An error in error analysis. Language Learning, 24 (2), 205-214.10.1111/j.1467-1770.1974.tb00502.xSearch in Google Scholar

Scott-Phillips, T.C. & Kirby, S. (2010). Language evolution in the laboratory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14 (9), 411-417.10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.006Search in Google Scholar

Seguy, J. (1973). La dialectom'etrie dans l’atlas linguistique de gascogne. Revue de Linguistique Romane, 37 (145), 1-24.Search in Google Scholar

Shennan, S. (2000). Population, culture history and the dynamics of culture change. Current Anthropology, 41, 811-835.10.1086/317403Search in Google Scholar

Smith, C. & Smith, D. (2003). Ingestion of ethanol just prior to sleep onset impairs memory for procedural but not declarative tasks. Sleep, 15 (26), 185-191.Search in Google Scholar

Smith, K. & Kirby, S. (2008). Cultural evolution: implications for understanding the human language faculty and its evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363 (1509), 3591-3603.Search in Google Scholar

Smith, K. & Wonnacott, E. (2010). Eliminating unpredictable variation through iterated learning. Cognition, 116 (3), 444-449.10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.004Search in Google Scholar

Tamariz, M., Cornish, H., Roberts, S., & Kirby, S. (2012). The effect of generation turnover and intelocutor negotiation on linguistic structure. In T.C. Phillips, M. Tamariz, E.A. Cartmill, & J.R. Hurford (Eds.), The evolution of language: Proceedings of the 9th International Conference EVOLANG9 (pp. 555-556). Singapore: World Scientific.Search in Google Scholar

Trudgill, P. (1974). The social differentiation of English in Norwich. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Trudgill, P. (2004). Linguistic and social typology: The Austronesian migrations and phoneme inventories. Linguistic Typology, 8, 305-320.10.1515/lity.2004.8.3.305Search in Google Scholar

Trudgill, P. (2011). Social structure and phoneme inventories. Linguistic Typology, 15, 155-160.10.1515/lity.2011.010Search in Google Scholar

Ullman, M. (2005). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on second language acqui­sition: The declarative/procedural model. In C. Sanz (Ed.), Mind and context in adult second language acquisition: Methods, theory, and practice (pp. 141-178). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Vogt, P. (2009). Modelling interactions between language evolution and demography. Human Biology, 81 (2-3), 237-258.10.3378/027.081.0307Search in Google Scholar

Wasow, T. & Arnold, J. (2005). Intuitions in linguistic argumentation. Lingua, 115, 1481-1496.10.1016/j.lingua.2004.07.001Search in Google Scholar

Weinreich, U., Labov, W., & Herzog, M. (1968). Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. W.P. Lehmann, Y. Malkiel (Eds.), Directions for historical linguistics: A symposium (pp. 97-195). Austin: University of Texas Press.Search in Google Scholar

Welsch, R.L., Terrell, J., & Nadolski, J.A. (1992). Language and culture on the north coast of new guinea. American Anthropologist, 94 (3), 568-600.10.1525/aa.1992.94.3.02a00030Search in Google Scholar

Wichmann, S. & Holman, E.W. (2009). Population size and rates of language change. Human Biology, 81 (2-3), 259-74.10.3378/027.081.0308Search in Google Scholar

Wichmann, S., Rama, T., & Holman, E. (2011). Phonological diversity, word length, and population sizes across languages: The ASJP evidence. Linguistic Typology, 15, 177-197.10.1515/lity.2011.013Search in Google Scholar

Wikipedia (2011). List of flags by number of colors. online, accessed 12-04-2011. <http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/list> of flags by number of colors.Search in Google Scholar

Winters, J. (2010). Phoneme inventory size and demography. In Proceedings of the 24th Language at Edinburgh Lunch, Edinburgh.Search in Google Scholar

World Values Survey Association (2009). World Values Survey 1981-2008 Official Aggregate v. 20090901. Madrid: ASEP/JDS.Search in Google Scholar

Wray, A. & Grace, G.W. (2007). The consequences of talking to strangers: Evolu­tionary corollaries of socio-cultural influences on linguistic form. Lingua, 117, 543-578.10.1016/j.lingua.2005.05.005Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
2083-8506
ISSN:
1234-2238
Language:
English
Publication timeframe:
Volume Open
Journal Subjects:
Social Sciences, Psychology, Applied Psychology