Cite

Anderson, J. R. (2007). How Can the Mind Occur in the Physical Universe? Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195324259.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 105-24-67. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0500003810.1017/S0140525X05000038 Search in Google Scholar

Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language: the mirror system hypothesis. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199896684.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-210.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 Search in Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working Memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). New York / San Francisco / London: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-110.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1 Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W. (1986). The Nature of Scientific Integration. In W. Bechtel (Ed.), Integrating Scientific Disciplines (pp. 3-52). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-9435-1 110.1007/978-94-010-9435-1 Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W. (2008). Mental Mechanisms. New York: Routledge (Taylor & Francis Group). Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W. (2016). Investigating neural representations: the tale of place cells. Synthese, 193(5), 1287-1321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0480-810.1007/s11229-014-0480-8 Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W., & McCauley, R. N. (1999). Heuristic identity theory (or back to the future): The mind-body problem against the background of research strategies in cognitive neuroscience. In Proceedings of the 21st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 67-72). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Search in Google Scholar

Benhamou, S. (1996). No evidence for cognitive mapping in rats. Animal Behaviour, 52(1), 201-212.10.1006/anbe.1996.0165 Search in Google Scholar

Bickle, J. (1998). Psychoneural reduction the new wave. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Boone,W., & Piccinini, G. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience revolution. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0783-410.1007/s11229-015-0783-4 Search in Google Scholar

Busemeyer, J. R., & Diederich, A. (2010). Cognitive modeling. Los Angeles: Sage. Search in Google Scholar

Byrne, M. D. (2012). Unified theories of cognition.Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(4), 431-438. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.118010.1002/wcs.1180 Search in Google Scholar

Chaitin, G. J. (1987). Algorithmic information theory. Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]; New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511608858 Search in Google Scholar

Churchland, P. M. (1985). Reduction, qualia, and the direct introspection of brain states. The Journal of Philosophy, 82(1), 8-28.10.2307/2026509 Search in Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2013).Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181-204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1200047710.1017/S0140525X12000477 Search in Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: prediction, action, and the embodied mind.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190217013.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Cooper, R. P. (2007). The Role of Falsification in the Development of Cognitive Architectures: Insights from a Lakatosian Analysis. Cognitive Science, 31(3), 509-533. https://doi.org/10.1080/1532690070132659210.1080/15326900701326592 Search in Google Scholar

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From Evolution to Behavior: Evolutionary Psychology as the Missing Link. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The Latest on the Best. Essays on Evolution and Optimality (pp. 277-303). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F. (2002). Interlevel experiments and multilevel mechanisms in the neuroscience of memory. Philosophy of Science, 69(S3), 83-97.10.1086/341836 Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F. (2007a). Explaining the Brain. Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199299317.003.0007 Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F. (2007b). Explaining the Brain. Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199299317.003.0007 Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F., & Darden, L. (2013). In search of mechanisms: discoveries across the life sciences.10.7208/chicago/9780226039824.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Cummins, R. (1984). Functional analysis. Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Biology: An Anthology. Search in Google Scholar

Cummins, R. (2000). “How does it work” versus “what are the laws?”: Two conceptions of psychological explanation. In F. Keil & R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and Cognition (pp. 117-145). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Danks, D. (2014). Unifying the mind: cognitive representations as graphical models. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9540.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Darden, L., & Maull, N. (1977). Interfield Theories. Philosophy of Science, 44(1), 43-64.10.1086/288723 Search in Google Scholar

Dawson, M. (1998). Understanding cognitive science. Malden Mass.: Blackwell. Search in Google Scholar

Derdikman, D., & Moser, E. I. (2010). A manifold of spatial maps in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 561-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.00410.1016/j.tics.2010.09.004 Search in Google Scholar

di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176-80.10.1007/BF00230027 Search in Google Scholar

Eliasmith, C. (2013). How to build the brain: a neural architecture for biological cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199794546.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Eliasmith, C., Stewart, T. C., Choo, X., Bekolay, T., DeWolf, T., Tang, Y., ... Rasmussen, D. (2012). A Large-Scale Model of the Functioning Brain. Science, 338(6111), 1202-1205. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.122526610.1126/science.1225266 Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. (1968). Psychological explanation: an introduction to the philosophy of psychology. New York: Random House. Search in Google Scholar

Forster, M., & Sober, E. (1994). How to Tell When Simpler, More Unified, or Less Ad Hoc Theories will Provide More Accurate Predictions. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 45(1), 1-35. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/45.1.110.1093/bjps/45.1.1 Search in Google Scholar

Gallese, V. (2003). The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity. Psychopathology, 36(4), 171-180. https://doi.org/10.1159/00007278610.1159/000072786 Search in Google Scholar

Glennan, S. S. (2002). Rethinking Mechanistic Explanation. Philosophy of Science, 69(S3), S342-S353. https://doi.org/10.1086/34185710.1086/341857 Search in Google Scholar

Goodman, N. (1961). Safety, Strength, Simplicity. Philosophy of Science, 28(2), 150-151. https://doi.org/10.1086/28779510.1086/287795 Search in Google Scholar

Hempel, C., & Oppenheim, P. (1948). Studies in the Logic of Explanation. Philosophy of Science, 15(2), 135-175.10.1086/286983 Search in Google Scholar

Hensel, W. M. (2013). On Reduction and Interfield Integration in Neuroscience. In M. Miłkowski & K. Talmont-Kamiński (Eds.), Regarding the Mind, Naturally: Naturalist Approaches to the Sciences of the Mental (pp. 167-181). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing. Search in Google Scholar

Hickok, G. (2014). The myth of mirror neurons: the real neuroscience of communication and cognition. New York: WW Norton. Search in Google Scholar

Hochstein, E. (2015). One mechanism, many models: a distributed theory of mechanistic explanation. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0844-810.1007/s11229-015-0844-8 Search in Google Scholar

Hooker, C. A. (1981a). Towards a General Theory of Reduction. Part I: Historical and Scientific Setting. Dialogue, 20(1), 38-59. https://doi.org/10.1017/S001221730002308810.1017/S0012217300023088 Search in Google Scholar

Hooker, C. A. (1981b). Towards a General Theory of Reduction. Part II: Identity in Reduction. Dialogue, 20(2), 201-236. https://doi.org/10.1017/S001221730002330110.1017/S0012217300023301 Search in Google Scholar

Hooker, C. A. (1981c). Towards a General Theory of Reduction. Part III: Cross- Categorical Reduction. Dialogue, 20(3), 496-529. https://doi.org/10.1017/S001221730002359310.1017/S0012217300023593 Search in Google Scholar

Hora, J., & Campos, P. (2015). A review of performance criteria to validate simulation models. Expert Systems, 32(5), 578-595. https://doi.org/10.1111/exsy.1211110.1111/exsy.12111 Search in Google Scholar

Kitcher, P. (1989). Explanatory Unification and the Causal Structure of the World. In P. Kitcher & W. C. Salmon (Eds.), Scientific Explanation (Vol. 505, pp. 410-505). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Search in Google Scholar

Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science (New York, N.Y.), 297(5582), 846-8. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.107031110.1126/science.1070311 Search in Google Scholar

Laird, J. E., Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P. S. (1987). SOAR: An architecture for general intelligence. Artificial Intelligence, 33(1), 1-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(87)90050-610.1016/0004-3702(87)90050-6 Search in Google Scholar

Li, M., & Vitanyi, P. (1993). An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and Its Applications. New York, Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.10.1007/978-1-4757-3860-5 Search in Google Scholar

McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, W. (1943). A Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in Nervous Activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 5, 115-133.10.1007/BF02478259 Search in Google Scholar

Metta, G., Natale, L., Nori, F., Sandini, G., Vernon, D., Fadiga, L., ... Montesano, L. (2010). The iCub humanoid robot: An open-systems platform for research in cognitive development. Neural Networks, 23(8), 1125-1134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.01010.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.010 Search in Google Scholar

Miłkowski,M. (2013). Explaining the Computational Mind. Cambridge,Mass.:MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9339.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Morse, A. F., Herrera, C., Clowes, R., Montebelli, A., & Ziemke, T. (2011). The role of robotic modelling in cognitive science. New Ideas in Psychology, 29(3), 312-324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.02.00110.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.02.001 Search in Google Scholar

Mouras, H., Stol´eru, S., Moulier, V., P´el´egrini-Issac, M., Rouxel, R., Grandjean, B., ... Bittoun, J. (2008). Activation of mirror-neuron system by erotic video clips predicts degree of induced erection: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 42, 1142-1150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.05110.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.051 Search in Google Scholar

Newell, A. (1973). You can’t play 20 questions with nature and win: Projective comments on the papers of this symposium. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 283-308). New York: Academic Press. Search in Google Scholar

Newell, A. (1990). Unified Theories of Cognition. Cambridge, Mass. and London: Harvard University Press. Search in Google Scholar

O’Reilly, R. C., & Munakata, Y. (2000). Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience. Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/2014.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Piccinini, G., & Craver, C. F. (2011). Integrating psychology and neuroscience: functional analyses as mechanism sketches. Synthese, 183(3), 283-311. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-011-9898-410.1007/s11229-011-9898-4 Search in Google Scholar

Ponseti, J., Bosinski, H. A., Wolff, S., Peller, M., Jansen, O., Mehdorn, H. M., ... Siebner, H. R. (2006). A functional endophenotype for sexual orientation in humans. NeuroImage, 33(3), 825-833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.00210.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.002 Search in Google Scholar

Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New Yorker, The. Hutchinson.10.1063/1.3060577 Search in Google Scholar

Quine, W. V. (1948). On What There Is. The Review of Metaphysics, 2(5), 21-38. Search in Google Scholar

Ramón y Cajal, S. (1990). New ideas on the structure of the nervous system in man and vertebrates. (L. W. Swanson, Trans.). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Richardson, R. C. (2007). Evolutionary Psychology as Maladapted Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/7464.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-92. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.14423010.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230 Search in Google Scholar

Roberts, S., & Pashler, H. (2000). How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing. Psychological Review, 107(2), 358.10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.358 Search in Google Scholar

Salmon, W. C. (1998). Causality and explanation. Oxford University Press, USA.10.1093/0195108647.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Schaffner, K. F. (1993). Discovery and explanation in biology and medicine. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Search in Google Scholar

Schurz, G. (1991). Relevant Deduction: From Solving Paradoxes Towards a General Theory. Erkenntnis, 35, 391-437. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF0038829510.1007/BF00388295 Search in Google Scholar

Thagard, P. (2000). Coherence in thought and action. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/1900.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Thagard, P. (2007). Coherence, Truth, and the Development of Scientific Knowledge. Philosophy of Science, 74, 28-47.10.1086/520941 Search in Google Scholar

Van Orden, G. C., & Kloos, H. (2003). The Module Mistake. Cortex, 39(1), 164-166. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70092-310.1016/S0010-9452(08)70092-3 Search in Google Scholar

Votsis, I. (2015). Unification: Not Just a Thing of Beauty. THEORIA. An International Journal for Theory, History and Foundations of Science, 30(1), 97. https://doi.org/10.1387/theoria.1269510.1387/theoria.12695 Search in Google Scholar

Weisberg, M. (2006). Robustness Analysis. Philosophy of Science, 73(5), 730-742. https://doi.org/10.1086/51862810.1086/518628 Search in Google Scholar

Zenil, H. (2010). Compression-based investigation of the dynamical properties of cellular automata and other systems. Journal of Complex Systems, 19(1).10.25088/ComplexSystems.19.1.1 Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
2199-6059
Language:
English
Publication timeframe:
4 times per year
Journal Subjects:
Philosophy, other