Open Access

The Embodied Nature of Autistic Learning: Implications for Physical Education


Cite

American Psychiatric Association (Eds.). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Retrieved March 11, 2015, from http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596Search in Google Scholar

Baker, M.J. (2000). Incorporating the thematic ritualistic behaviors of children with autism into games: increasing social play interactions with siblings. J. Posit. Behav. Intervent., 2, 66-84.10.1177/109830070000200201Search in Google Scholar

Barnes, K.A., Howard, J.H.Jr., Howard, D.V., Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Gaillard, W.D., Vaidya, C.J. (2008). Intact implicit learning of spatial context and complex sequences in childhood autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychology, 22(5), 563-570.10.1037/0894-4105.22.5.563Search in Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M. & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37-46.10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8Search in Google Scholar

Bodfish, J.W. (2007). Stereotypy, Self-Injury, and Related Abnormal Repetitive Behaviors. In Jacobson, J.W., Mulick, J.A. & Rojahn, J. (Eds.), Handbook of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (pp. 481-506). Berlin: Springer Science + Business Media.Search in Google Scholar

Brown, J., Aczel, B., Jiménez, L., Kaufman, S.B. & Grant, K.P. (2010). Intact implicit learning in autism spectrum conditions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63(9), 1789-1812. DOI:10.1080/17470210903536910.10.1080/1747021090353691020204919Search in Google Scholar

Carruthers, P. (1996). Simulation and self-knowledge: A defense of theory-theory. In Carruthers, P. & Smith, P.K. (Eds.), Theories of Theories of Mind (pp. 22-38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Connolly, M. (2008). The remarkable logic of autism: developing and describing an embedded curriculum based in semiotic phenomenology. Sport, Ethics and Philosophy, 2(2), 234-256.10.1080/17511320802223824Search in Google Scholar

De Jaegher, H. (2013). Embodiment and sense-making in autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, Article 15.10.3389/fnint.2013.00015360780623532205Search in Google Scholar

De Jaegher, H., Di Paulo, E.A. & Gallagher, S. (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 441-447.10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.00920674467Search in Google Scholar

Dickerson, P., Stribling, P. & Rae, J. (2007). Tapping into interaction: how children with autistic spectrum disorders design and place tapping in relation to activities in progress. Gesture, 7, 271-303.10.1075/gest.7.3.02dicSearch in Google Scholar

Dreyfus, H.L. (2002). Intelligence without representation - Merleau-Ponty‟s critique of mental representation. Phenomenology and Cognitive Science, 1, 367-383.10.1023/A:1021351606209Search in Google Scholar

Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the Enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Fuchs, T. (2010). Phenomenology and Psychopathology. In Gallagher, S., & Schmiching, D. (Eds.), Handbook of Phenomenology and Cognitive Science (pp. 547-573). Berlin: Springer Science + Business Media.Search in Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (1992). Hermeneutics and Education. Albany: State University of New York Press.Search in Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (2004). Understanding interpersonal problems in autism: interaction theory as an alternative to theory of mind. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 11(3), 199-217.10.1353/ppp.2004.0063Search in Google Scholar

Gallagher, S. (2005). How the Body Shapes the Mind. Oxford: Clarendon Press.10.1093/0199271941.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Gepner, B. & Mestre, D. (2002). Rapid visual-motion integration deficit in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 455.10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02004-1Search in Google Scholar

Gipps, R.G.T. (2004). Autism and intersubjectivity: beyond cognitivism and the theory of mind. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 11(3), 195-198.10.1353/ppp.2004.0065Search in Google Scholar

Goldman, A.I. (2012). Theory of Mind. In Margolis, E., Samuels, R. & Stich, S. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy of Cognitive Science (pp. 402-424). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Happé, F. (1995). Autism: An Introduction to Psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Hobson, R.P. (1990). On the origins of self and the case of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 163-181.10.1017/S0954579400000687Search in Google Scholar

Hobson, R.P. (1993). Autism and the Development of Mind. Howe, Sussex: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Hobson, R.P. (2010). Explaining autism: Ten reasons to focus on the developing self. Autism, 14(5), 391-407.10.1177/136236131036414220926456Search in Google Scholar

Hung, D. (1978). Using self-stimulation as reinforcement for autistic children. J. Autism Child Schizophr., 8, 355-366.10.1007/BF01539637690069Search in Google Scholar

Jensen, K.S. (2014). Kropslig situeret læring. Et casestudie af læring i idrætsundervisningen for elever med ASF på Taps skole /Bodily situated learning: a case study of learning in physical education for pupils with ASD at Taps School/. Aarhus: Department of Public Health, Aarhus University.Search in Google Scholar

Liss, M., Saulnier, C., Fein, D. & Kinsbourne, M. (2006). Sensory and attention abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 10, 155-172.10.1177/136236130606202116613865Search in Google Scholar

Merleau-Ponty, M. (2012). Phenomenology of Perception. Oxon: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Mottron, L., Burack, J., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S. & Enns, J. (2003) Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(6), 904-913.10.1111/1469-7610.0017412959498Search in Google Scholar

Nadel, J. (2014). How Imitation Boosts Development. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

O'Loughlin, M. (1996). Embodiment and Education: Exploring Creatural Existence. Berlin: Springer.Search in Google Scholar

Reber, A.S., Walkenfeld, F.F. & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(5), 888-896.Search in Google Scholar

Reed, T. & Paterson, C. (1990). A comparative study of autistic subjects‟ performance at two levels of visual and cognitive perspective taking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 555-68.10.1007/BF022160602279973Search in Google Scholar

Spiker, M.A., Lin, C.E., Van Dyke, M. & Wood, J.J. (2012). Restricted interests and anxiety in children with autism. Autism, 16, 306-320.10.1177/136236131140176321705474Search in Google Scholar

Stanghellini, G. & Ballerini, G. (2004). Autism: Disembodied existence. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 11(3), 260.Search in Google Scholar

Winch, C. (2001). The Philosophy of Human Learning. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Zager, D. (2013). Positive Psychology and Autism Spectrum Disorders. In Wehmeyer, M.L. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology and Disability (pp. 494-505). Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195398786.013.013.0030Search in Google Scholar

Zahavi, D. & Parnas, J. (2003). Conceptual problems in infantile autism research: why cognitive science needs phenomenology. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(9-10), 53-71. Search in Google Scholar