“Storyline” or “Associations Pyramid”? A Relationship Between the Difficulty of Educational Methods and Their Effectiveness in Developing Language Creativity Among Pre-School Children

Open access

Abstract

This article presents the results of a comparison of two educational methods – the “Storyline” and the “Associations Pyramid” – in developing language creativity among children. The methods were compared in terms of effectiveness with two post-tests, directly after the end of the experiment and after the next three months. Moreover, the initial level of operational thinking (from the pre-test) was used in a regression model as an independent variable to observe whether it predicts results in the language creativity of children in both groups, in post-test 1. Eighty-three preschoolers took part in the experimental study. The two methods do not differ significantly from each other in effectiveness. Also, the level of operational thinking does not predict an overall level of language creativity either in the “Storyline” group or in the “Associations Pyramid” group. The results are discussed in the light of pedagogical practice.

If the inline PDF is not rendering correctly, you can download the PDF file here.

  • Bandura A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. New Jersey NJ: Prentice Hall.

  • Barrett M. S. (2006). Inventing songs inventing worlds: the ‘genesis’ of creativity thoughts and activity in young children’s lives. International Journal of Early Years Education14 201–220. doi: 10.1080/09669760600879920

  • Barnett W. S. Jung K. Yarosz D. J. Thomas J. Hornbeck A. & Burns S. (2008). Educational effects of the Tools of the Mind curriculum: A randomized trial. Early Childhood Research Quarterly23 299–313. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.03.001

  • Bates E. Dale P. & Thal D. (1996). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In P. Fletcher B. McWhinney (Eds.) The handbook of child language (pp. 96–151). Oxford: Blackwell.

  • Bayliss D. M. Jarrold Ch. Baddelley A. D. & Leigh E. (2005). Differential constraints on the working memory and reading abilities of individuals with learning difficulties and typically developing children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology92 76–99. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2005.04.002

  • Bayliss D. M. Jarrold C. Gunn D. M. & Baddelley A. D. (2003). The complexities of complex span: Explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General132 71–92. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.71

  • Bell S. & Harkness S. (2006). Storyline – promoting language across the curriculum. Royston: UKLA.

  • Belletheim B. (2010). The uses of enchantment: The meaning and importance of fairy tales. New York NY: Vintage Books.

  • Belsky J. & de Haan M. (2011). Annual Research Review: Parenting and children’s brain development: the end of the beginning. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry52 409–428. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02281.x

  • Besharov D. J. (2005). Head Start’s broken promise. Washington DC: American Enterprise Institute.

  • Bivens J. A. & Berk L. A. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children’s private speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly36 443–463.

  • Bokus B. (1992). Peer co-narration: Changes in structure of preschoolers' participation. Journal of Narrative and Life History2 253–275. doi:10.1075/jnlh.2.3.05pee

  • Bokus B. (1996). Narrative space structuring at the preschool age. Findings on monologic and dialogic discourse. In C. E. Johnson & J. H. V. Gilbert (Eds.) Children’s language vol. 9 (pp. 197–207). Mahvah NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Borman G. D. Slavin R. E. Cheung A. Chamberlain A. Madden N. & Chambers B. (2007). Final reading outcomes of the national randomized field trial of Success for All. Madison WI: University of Wisconsin-Madison.

  • Bronfenbrenner U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology22 723–742. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723

  • Bruner J. (1995). From Joint Attention to the Meeting of Minds. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.) Joint Attention. Its origins and role in development (pp. 1–15). New Jersey NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

  • Bruner J. (2010). The culture of education. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Burger K. (2010). How does early childhood care and education affect cognitive development? An international review of the effects of early interventions for children from different social backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly25 140–165. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.11.001

  • Burke L. A. & Williams J. M. (2008). Developing Young Thinkers: An intervention aimed to enhance children’s thinking skills. Thinking Skills and Creativity3 104–124. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2008.01.001

  • Camilli G. Vargas S. Ryan S. & Barnett S. W. (2010). Meta-analysis of the effects of early education interventions on cognitive and social development. Teachers College Records112 579–620.

  • Cannella G. S. (1993). Learning through social interaction: Shared cognitive experience negotiation strategies and joint concept construction for young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly8 427–444. doi:10.1016/S0885-2006(05)80078-X

  • Dewey J. (1997). Experience and education. New York NY: Touchstone.

  • Doise W. (1990). The development of individual competencies trough social interaction. In H. C. Foot M. J. Morgan & R. H. Shute (Eds.) Children helping children (pp. 43–64). Chichester: J. Wiley and sons.

  • Donaldson M. (1987). Children’s minds. London UK: Fontana Press.

  • Druyan S. (2001). A comparison of four types of cognitive conflict and their effect on cognitive development. International Journal of Behavioral Development25 226–36. doi:10.1080/01650250042000276

  • Duncan G. J. Dowsett C. J. Claessens A. Magnuson K. Huston A. C. Klebanov P. Pagani L. S. Feinstein L. Engel M. Brooks-Gunn J. Sexton H. Duckworth K. & Japel C. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology43 1428–1446. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428

  • Dziedziewicz D. Oledzka D. & Karwowski M. (2013). Developing 4- to 6-year-old children's figural creativity using a doodle-book program. Thinking Skills and Creativity9 85–95. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2012.09.004

  • Edwards C. Gandini L. & Forman G. (1998). The hundred languages of children. London: Ablex Publishing.

  • Ely R. & McCabe A. (1994). The language play in kindergarten children. First Language14 19–35. doi:10.1177/014272379401404202

  • Flavell J. H. (1978). Developmental stage: Explanans or explanandum? Behavioural and Brain Sciences2 187–188. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00073908

  • Flynn J. R. (2000). IQ trends over time: Intelligence race and meritocracy. In W. Arrow S. Bowles & S. Durlauf (Eds.) Meritocracy and economic inequality (pp. 35–60). Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.

  • Gauvain M. (2001). The social context of cognitive development. New York NY: Guilford.

  • Geist E. & Aldridge J. (2002). The developmental progression of children’s oral story inventions. Journal of Instructional Psychology29 33–39.

  • Gentner D. & Boroditksy L. (2001). Individuation relativity and early word learning. In: M. Bowerman & S. Levinson (Eds.) Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 215–256). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Glaveanu V.P. (2011). Children and creativity: A most (un)likely pair? Thinking Skills and Creativity6 122–131. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2011.03.002

  • Glenn-Applegate K. Breit-Smith A. & Justice L. M. (2010). Artfulness in young children’s spoken narratives. Early Education and Development21 468–493. doi:10.1080/10409280903440620

  • Gopnik A. & Meltzoff A. (1997). Words thoughts and theories. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press.

  • Gormley W. T. Jr. Gayer T. Philips D. & Dawson B. (2005). The effects of universal pre-K on cognitive development. Developmental Psychology41 872–884. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.872

  • Granpeesheh D. Dixon D. R. Tarbox J. Kaplan A. M. & Wilke A. E. (2009). The effects of age and treatment intensity on behavioral intervention outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders3 1014–1022. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2009.06.007

  • Hamre B. K. & Pianta R. (2005). Can instructional and emotional support in the first-grade classroom make a difference for children at risk of school failure? Child Development76 949–967. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00889.x

  • Hattie J. (2010). Visible Learning. A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses relating to achievments. London: Routledge.

  • Hickmann M. (2001). Language and cognition in development: Old questions new directions. Pragmatics11 105–126. doi:10.1075/prag.11.2.01hic

  • Hill J. L. Brooks-Gunn J. & Waldfogel J. (2003). Sustained effects of high participation in an early intervention for low birth-weight premature infants. Developmental Psychology39 730–744. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.4.730

  • Howard-Jones P. Taylor J. Sutton L. (2002). The effect of play on the creativity of young children during subsequent activity. Early Child Development and Care172 323–328. doi:10.1080/03004430212722

  • Howe Ch. (2010). Peer groups and children’s development. Chichester: Wiley- Blackwell.

  • Kazdin A. E. & Weisz J. R. (2003) (Eds.) Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents. New York NY: Guilford Press.

  • Kujawski J. (2000). Zarys programu pracy z młodzieżą zdolną. In M. Partyka (Ed.) Modele opieki nad dzieckiem zdolnym [Care models for able child] (pp. 130–137). Warszawa: Centrum Metodyczne Pomocy Psychologiczno- Pedagogicznej Ministerstwa Edukacji Narodowej.

  • La Paro K. M. & Pianta R. C. (2000). Predicting children’s competence in the early school years: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research70 443–484. doi:10.3102/00346543070004443

  • Leaper C. & Smith T. E. (2004). A meta-analytic review of gender variations in children’s language use: Talkativeness affiliative speech and assertive speech. Developmental Psychology40 993–1027. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.993

  • Lepola J. Niemi P. Kuikka M. & Hannula M. M. (2005). Cognitive- linguistics skills and motivation as longitudinal predictors of reading and arithmetic achievement: A follow-up study from kindergarten to grade 2. International Journal of Educational Research43 250–271. doi:10.1016/j.ijer.2006.06.005

  • Lieven E. & Tomasello M. (2008). Children’s first language acquisition from a usage-based perspective. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (Eds.) Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition (pp. 168–196). New York NY: Routledge.

  • Lillard A. S. (2012). Preschool children’s development in classic Montessori suplemented Montessori and conventional programs. Journal of School Psychology50 379–401. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2012.01.001

  • Love J. M. Kisker E. E. Ross C. Raikes H. Constantine J. Boller K. Brooks- Gunn J. Chazan-Cohen R. Tarullo L. B. Brady-Smith C. Fuligni A. S. Schochet P. Z. Paulsell D. & Vogel C. (2005). The effectiveness of Early Head Start for 3-year-old children and their parents: Lessons for policy and programs. Developmental Psychology41 885–901.

  • Lyons J. (1977). Semantics vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Maker C. J. Jo S. & Muammar O. M. (2008). Development of creativity: The influence of varying levels of implementation of the DISCOVER curriculum model a non-traditional pedagogical approach. Learning and Individual Differences18 402–417. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2008.03.003

  • Matczak A. (2001). Test Operacyjności Myślenia: Diagnoza możliwości intelektualnych dziecka. Podręcznik [Operational Thinking Test. A diagnosis of child’s intellectual capabilities. A Manual]. Warszawa PL: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych Polskiego Towarzystwa Psychologicznego.

  • Mellou E. (1995). Review of the relationship between dramatic play and creativity in young children. Early Child Development and Care112 85–107. doi:10.1080/0300443951120108

  • Murray C. (2002). IQ and income inequality in a sample of sibling pairs from advantages family backgrounds. Paper at 114th Annual American Economic Association meeting Atlanta GA.

  • Nicolopoulou A. & Richner E. (2004). ‘When your power combine I am Capitan Planet’: the developmental significance of individual- and group-authored stories by preschoolers. Discourse Studies6 347–371. doi:10.1177/1461445604044293

  • Nisbett R. E. (2009). Intelligence and how to get it. Why schools and culture count. New York NY: W. W. Norton & Co.

  • Olson K. R. & Spelke E. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition108 222–231. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003

  • Panselina G. & Komis V. (2009). ‘Scaffolding’ through talk in groupwork learning. Thinking Skills and Creativity4 86–103. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2009.06.002

  • Papafragou A. Li P. Choi Y. & Han Ch. (2007). Evidentiality in language and cognition. Cognition103 253–299. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.001

  • Pederson E. Danziger E. Wilkins D. Levinson S. Kita S. & Senft G. (1998). Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization. Language74 557–589.

  • Peterson C. & McCabe A. E. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child’s narrative. New York NY: Plenum Press.

  • Piaget J. (2001). The language and thought of the child. London: Routledge Classics.

  • Rhoades B. L. Warren H. K. Domitrovich C. E. & Greenberg M. T. (2011). Examining the link between preschool social-emotional competence and first grade academic achievement: The role of attention skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly26 182–191. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.07.003

  • Rogoff B. Mistry J. Gӧncü A. & Mosier C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development58(236). doi:10.2307/1166109

  • Russ S. (2003). Play and creativity: Developmental issues. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research47 291–303. doi:10.1080/00313830308594

  • Saracho O. (2002). Young children’s creativity and pretend play. Early Child Development and Care172 431–438. doi:10.1080/03004430214553

  • Saxton M. (2010). Child language. Acquisition and development. London: Sage.

  • Schaffer H. R. (2006). Key concepts in developmental psychology. London: Sage.

  • Schweinhart L. J. Montie J. Xiang Z. Barnett W. S. Belfield C. R. & Nores M. (2005). Lifetime effects: The High/Scope Perry Preschool Study through age 40. Ypsilanti MI: High Scope Press.

  • Sheridan S. & Williams P. (2006). Collaboration as One Aspect of Quality: A perspective of collaboration and pedagogical quality in educational settings. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research50 83–93. doi:10.1080/00313830500372067

  • Slavin R. E. (2005). Show me the evidence: Effective programs for elementary and secondary schools. Baltimore MD: Johns Hopkins University Center for Data Driven Reform in Education.

  • Smogorzewska J. (2014). Developing children’s language creativity through telling stories – An experimental study. Thinking Skills and Creativity13 20–31. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2014.02.005

  • Stein M. I. (1953). Creativity and culture. Journal of Psychology36 311–322. doi:10.1080/00223980.1953.9712897

  • Stein N. & Glenn C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.) New directions in discourse processing. Vol. 2 (pp. 53–120). New York NY: Ablex.

  • Sweller J. (2008). Cognitive load theory and the use of educational technology. Educational Technology48 32–34.

  • Tolmie A. Howe C. Mackenzie M. & Greer K. (1993). Task design as an influence on dialogue and learning: primary school group work with object flotation. Social Development2 183–201. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.1993.tb00013.x

  • Tomasello M. (2001). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Tomasello M. (2009). Why we cooperate. London: The MIT Press.

  • Turiel E. (2010). Domain specificity in social interactions social thought and social development. Child Development3 720–726. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01429.x

  • Veneziano E. & Huelot Ch. (2009). Explaining events in narratives: The impact of scaffolding in 4 to 12 year old children. Psychology of Language and Communication13(1) 4–20. doi:10.2478/v10057-009-0001-X

  • Vygotsky L. S. (1966). Development of the higher mental functions. In A. N. Leont’ev A. R. Luria & A. Smirnov (Eds.) Psychological research in the USSR. Moscow: Progress.

  • Vygotsky L. S. (1986). Thought and language. London: The MIT Press.

  • Wenner J. A. Burch M. M. Lynch J. S. & Bauer P. J. (2008). Becoming a teller of tales. Associations between children’s fictional narratives and parent-child reminiscence narratives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology101 1–19. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2007.10.006

  • Wertsch J. V. & Tulviste P. (1992). L. S. Vygotsky and contemporary developmental psychology. Developmental Psychology28 548–557. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.4.548

  • Wood D. (1998). How children think and learn. The social contexts of cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

  • Wood D. Bruner J. & Ross G. (1976). The role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry17 89–100. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1976.tb00381.x

Search
Journal information
Impact Factor


CiteScore 2018: 0.29

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2018: 0.118
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2018: 0.410

Metrics
All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 0 0 0
Full Text Views 172 172 23
PDF Downloads 110 110 15