Intersubjectivity and the domains of social interaction: proposal of a cross-sectional approach

Open access

Abstract

Intersubjectivity is a concept central to human interaction, broadly understood as the sharing of minds. There is a rich diversity of conceptualizations of intersubjectivity, but detailed operationalization for its component processes in social interactions are scarce. We propose a novel approach to examine detailed variation in intersubjectivity in interaction. Our approach combines two previously formulated frameworks: the hierarchically organized developmental levels of intersubjectivity put forth in the field of developmental psychology, and three domains or orders of social interaction - affect, deontics, and epistemics - discussed in conversation analytic research literature. The interdisciplinary integration of these two frameworks allows a more crystallized view of intersubjectivity, which will benefit our understanding of the fine-scale social interaction processes as they vary in the course of the moment-to-moment unfolding of social action, across different stages of human social development, and between individuals belonging to different clinical groups and even to different species.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Akhtar, N., & Gernsbacher, M.A. (2007). Joint attention and vocabulary development: A critical look. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1, 195-207. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00014.x

Atkinson, J. M., & Drew, P. (1979). Order in court: The organisation of verbal interaction in judicial settings. London, UK: Macmillan.

Bard, K. A. (2012). Emotional engagement: How chimpanzee minds develop. In F. B. M. de Waal & P. F. Ferrari (Eds.), The primate mind built to connect with other minds (pp. 224-245). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(2), 139-148. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987. tb01049.x

Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (2008). The neuroscience of social understanding. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 39-66). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Bigelow, A. E., & Rochat, P. (2006). Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to social contingency in mother-infant and stranger-infant interaction. Infancy, 9, 313-325. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0903_3

Bowlby, J. (1982 [1969]). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1 (2nd. ed.). New York, NY: Basic Books.

Brinck, I. (2008). The role of intersubjectivity in the development of intentional communication. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 115-140). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Brosnan, S. F., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2014). Evolution of responses to (un)fairness. Science, 346, 1251776-1251776. doi: 10.1126/science.1251776

Bruner, J. (1985). The role of interaction formats in language acquisition. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Language and Social Situations (pp. 31-46). New York, NY: Springer.

Bullinger, A. F., Zimmermann, F., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Different social motives in the gestural communication of chimpanzees and human children. Developmental Science, 14, 58-68. doi: 10.1111/j.1467- 7687.2010.00952.x

Bühler, K. (2011 [1934]). Theory of language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task. PloS One, 12, e0173793-13. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0173793

Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187-192. doi: 10.1016/j. tics.2008.02.010

Carpenter, M., & Liebal, K. (2011). Joint attention, communication, and knowing together in infancy. In A. Seemann (Ed.), Joint attention: New developments in psychology, philosophy of mind, and social neuroscience (pp. 159-181). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Carpenter, M., & Call, J. (2013). How joint is the joint attention of apes and human infants? In H. S. Terrace & J. Metcalfe (Eds.), Agency and joint attention (pp. 49-61). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 76, 893-910. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893

Clark, H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Clay, Z., Ravaux, L., de Waal, F. B. M., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2016). Bonobos (Pan paniscus) vocally protest against violations of social expectations. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 130, 44-54. doi: 10.1037/a0040088

Coelho, N., & Figueiredo, L. C. (2003). Patterns of intersubjectivity in the constitution of subjectivity: Dimensions of otherness. Culture & Psychology, 9, 193-208. doi: 10.1177/1354067X030093002

Collins, R. (2004). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Condon, W. S. (1979). Neonatal entrainment and enculturation. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech (pp. 131-148). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Crosley, N. (1996). Intersubjectivity: The fabric of social becoming. London: Sage.

Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141-168. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0017.2009.01384.x

Darwall, S. L. (2006). The second-person standpoint: Morality, respect, and accountability. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 775-803. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000370

De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and Cognitive Sciences, 6, 485-507. doi: 10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9

De Jaegher, H., Peräkylä, A., & Stevanovic, M. (2016). The co-creation of meaningful action: Bridging enaction and interactional sociology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150378. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0378

Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? Developmental Psychology, 35, 418-426. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.418

Duranti, A. (2010). Husserl, intersubjectivity and anthropology. Anthropological Theory, 10(1-2), 16-35. doi: 10.1177/1463499610370517

Durkheim, É. (2001 [1912]). The elementary forms of religious life (translated by Carol Cosman). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Ekberg, K., & LeCouteur, A. (2015). Clients’ resistance to therapists’ proposals: Managing epistemic and deontic status. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 12-25. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2015.10.004

Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M., Lévesque, M., & Hess U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory: Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion, 7, 131-146. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.131

Enfield, N. J., & Stivers, T. (Eds.) (2007). Person reference in interaction: Linguistic, cultural, and social perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Enfield, N. J. (2006). Social consequences of common ground. In N. J. Enfield & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality (pp. 399-430). Oxford: Berg.

Enfield, N., & Sidnell, J. (2017). On the concept of action in the study of interaction. Discourse Studies, 19(5), 515-535. doi: 10.1177/1461445617730235

Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly- Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 445-470. doi: 10.2307/1130328

Fröhlich, M., Kuchenbuch, P., Müller, G., Fruth, B., Furuichi, T., Wittig, R. M., & Pika, S. (2016). Unpeeling the layers of language: Bonobos and chimpanzees engage in cooperative turn-taking sequences. Scientific Reports, 1-14. doi: 10.1038/srep25887

Fuchs, T., & De Jaegher, H. (2009). Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and Cognitive Sciences, 8, 465-486. doi: 10.1007/s11097-009-9136-4

Fusaroli, R., Gangopadhyay, N., & Tylén, K. (2014). The dialogically extended mind: Language as skillful intersubjective engagement. Cognitive Systems Research, 29-30, 31-39. doi: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2013.06.002

Gallagher, S., & Hutto, D. D. (2008). Understanding others through primary interaction and narrative practice. In Jordan Zlatev, Timothy P. Racine, Chris Sinha, & Esa Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 17-38). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Gallagher, S. (2011). Strong interaction and self-agency. Humana-Mente: Journal of Philosophical Studies, 15, 1-22.

Gergely, G., & Watson, J. S. (1999). Early social-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social biofeedback model. In P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition (pp. 101-136). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual: Essays on face-to-face behavior. Garden City: Doubleday.

Gräfenhain, M., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Three-year-olds’ understanding of the consequences of joint commitments. PLoS One, 8, e73039. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073039

Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. 2012: Children's developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development, 83, 137-145. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01695.x

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotion Contagion. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Heidegger, M. (1962 [1927]). Being and time. Oxford: Blackwell.

Heinemann, T. (2006). ”Will you or can't you?”: Displaying entitlement in interrogative requests. Journal of Pragmatics, 38(7), 1081-1104. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2005.09.013

Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Heritage, J. (2011). Territories of knowledge, territories of experience: Empathic moments in interaction. In T. Stivers, L. Mondada, & J. Steensig (Eds.), The morality of knowledge in conversation (pp. 159-183). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Heritage, J., & Raymond, G. (2005). The terms of agreement: Indexing epistemic authority and subordination in talk-in-interaction. Social Psychology Quarterly, 68, 15-38. doi: 10.1177/019027250506800103

Hobson, R. P., & Meyer J. A. (2005). Foundations for self and other: A study in autism. Developmental Science, 8, 481-491. doi: 10.1111/j.1467- 7687.2005.00439.x

Hobson, P., & Hobson J. A. (2008). Engaging, sharing, knowing: Some lessons from research in autism. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 67-88). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Hochschild, A. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules, and social structure. American Journal of Sociology, 85, 551-575. doi: 10.1086/227049

Hochschild, A. (1983). The managed heart. Berkeley: University of California.

Hornick, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937-944. doi: 10.2307/1130534

Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2010). The early development of object knowledge: a study of infants' visual anticipations during action observation. Developmental Psychology, 46, 446-454. doi: 10.1037/a0016543

Issartel, J., Marin, L., & Cadopi, M. (2007). Unintended interpersonal coordination: “Can we march to the beat of our own drum?” Neuroscience Letters, 411(3), 174-179. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.086

Jakobson, R. (1960). Closing statement: Linguistics and poetics. In T. A. Sebeok (Ed.), Style in language (pp. 350-377). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Johnson, D. B. (1983). Self-recognition in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 211-222. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(83)80028-9 Jordan, J. J., McAuliffe, K., & Warneken, F. (2014). Development of in-group favoritism in children’s third-party punishment of selfishness. PNAS, 111(35), 12710-12715. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1402280111

Kano, F., Hirata, S., Deschner, T., Behringer, V., & Call, J. (2016). Nasal temperature drop in response to a playback of conspecific fights in chimpanzees: A thermo-imaging study. Physiology & Behavior, 155, 83-94. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.029

Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., & Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affect sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 87-100. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.029

Keller, H., & Otto, H. (2009). The cultural socialization of emotion regulation during infancy. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(6), 996-1011. doi: 10.1177/0022022109348576

Kärtner, J., Borke, J., Maasmeier, K., Keller, H., & Kleis, A. (2011). Sociocultural influences on the development of self-recognition and self-regulation in Costa Rican and Mexican toddlers. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 10(1), 96-112. doi: 10.1891/1945-8959.10.1.96

Knorr-Cetina, K. (1999). Epistemic cultures: How the sciences make knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110-114. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf8110

Lagattuta, K. H. (2005). When you shouldn’t do what you want to do: Young children’s understanding of desires, rules, and emotions. Child Development, 76, 713-733. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00873.x

Landmark, A. M. D., Gulbrandsen, P. & Svennevig, J. (2015). Whose decision?: Negotiating epistemic and deontic rights in medical treatment decisions. Journal of Pragmatics, 78, 54-69. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2014.11.007

Leavens, D., & Racine, T. P. (2009). Joint attention in apes and humans: Are humans unique? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16, 240-267.

Leavens, D., Hopkins, W. D., & Bard, K. A. (2005). Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing. Epigenesis and ecological validity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 185-189. doi: 10.1111/j.0963- 7214.2005.00361.x

Lindström, A., & Weatherall, A. (2015). Orientations to epistemics and deontics in treatment discussions. Journal of Pragmatics, 78, 39-53. doi: 10.1016/j. pragma.2015.01.005

Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Twelve-and 18-month-olds point to provide information. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 173-187. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_2

Low, J., & Perner, J. (2012). Implicit and explicit theory of mind: State of the art. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 1-13. doi: 10.1111/j.2044- 835X.2011.02074.x

Macbeth, D. (1991). Teacher authority as practical action. Linguistics & Education, 3(4), 281-313. doi: 10.1016/0898-5898(91)90012-8

Malloch, S. N. (2000). Mothers and infants and communicative musicality. Musicae Scientiae, 2, 29-58. doi: 10.1177/10298649000030S104

Marsh, A. A., Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady N. (2003). “Nonverbal ‘Accents’: Cultural Differences in Facial Expressions of Emotion.” Psychological Science, 14, 373-76. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.24461

Marshall, P. J., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2014). Neural mirroring mechanisms and imitation in human infants. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130620-20130620. doi: 10.1098/ rstb.2013.0620

Maynard, D. W., & Freese, J. (2012). Good news, bad news, and affect: Practical and temporal “emotion work” in everyday life. In A. Peräkylä & M.-L. Sorjonen (Eds.), Emotion in interaction (pp. 92-112). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Melinder, A., Forbes D, Tronick E, Fikke, L., & Gredebäck G. (2010). The development of the still-face effect: Mothers do matter. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 472-481. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.05.003

Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as a building block for understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In B. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social science (pp. 171-191). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75-78. doi: 10.1126/science.198.4312.75

Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of Perception. London: Routledge.

Messman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2009). The many faces of the Still-Face Paradigm: A review and meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 29, 120-162. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.02.001

Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177-197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishers.

Nadel, J., Croué, S., Mattlinger, M.-J., Canet, P., Hudelot, C., Lécuyer, C, & Martini, M. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 164-173. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00065

Nagy, E., Compagne, H., Orvos, H., Pal, A. P., Molnar, P., Janszky, I., et al. (2005). Index finger movement imitation by human neonates: Motivation, learning and left-hand preference. Pediatric Research, 58, 749-753. doi: 10.1111/1467-7687.00065

Nesdale, D. (2008). Peer group rejection and children’s intergroup prejudice. In S. R. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood (pp. 32-46). Oxford: University Press.

Nielsen, M., Suddendorf, T., & Slaughter, V. (2006). Mirror self-recognition beyond the face. Child Development, 77, 176-185. doi: 10.1111/j.1467- 8624.2006.00863.x

Nomikou, I., & Rohlfing, K. J. (2011). Language does something: Body action and language in maternal input to three-month-olds. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 3(2), 113-128. doi: 10.1109/ TAMD.2011.2140113

Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., Redshaw, J., Kennedy-Costantini, S., Davis, J., Clark S, Slaughter V. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26, 1-25. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047

Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants. First Language, 9(6), 137-157. doi: 10.1177/014272378900900603

Papoušek, M., Papoušek, H., & Symmes, D. (1991). The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. Infant Behavior and Development, 14, 415-440. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(91)90031-M

Parr, L. A. (2001). Cognitive and physiological markers of emotional awareness in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), Animal Cognition, 4, 223-229. doi: 10.1007/s100710100085

Praetorius, N. (2010). Intersubjectivity, cognition, and language. In D. Schmicking & S. Gallagher (Eds.), Handbook of phenomenology and cognitive science (pp. 301-316). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands.

Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., Nomikou, I., & Rohlfing, K. (2013). Young children’s dialogical actions: The beginnings of purposeful intersubjectivity. IEEE Transactions in Autonomous Mental Development, 5(3), 210-221. doi: 10.1109/TAMD.2013.2273258

Rae, J. (1994). Social fax: Repair-mechanisms and intersubjectivity. American Behavioral Scientist, 37, 824-838. doi: 10.1177/0002764294037006008

Rakoczy, H. (2012). Do infants have a theory of mind? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 59-74. doi: 10.1111/j.2044- 835X.2011.02061.x

Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The sources of normativity: Young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology, 44, 875-881. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.875

Reddy, V. (2003). On being the object of attention: Implications for self-other consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 397-402. doi: 10.1016/ S1364-6613(03)00191-8

Reddy, V., Markova, G., & Wallot, S. (2013). Anticipatory adjustments to being picked up in infancy. PloS One, 8, e65289. doi: 10.1371/journal. pone.0065289

Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). No third-party punishment in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(37), 14824-14829. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1203179109

Rietveld, E. (2008). Situated normativity: The normative aspect of embodied cognition in unreflective action. Mind, 117, 973-1001. doi: 10.1093/mind/ fzn050

Rochat, P. (2001). The Infant’s World. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2000). Perceived self in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 23(3-4), 513-530. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00055-8

Rochat, P. (2007). Intentional action arises from early reciprocal exchanges. Acta Psychologica, 124, 8-25. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.004

Rochat, P., & Passos-Ferreira, C. (2009). From imitation to reciprocation and mutual recognition. In J. A. Pineda (Ed.), Mirror neurons system: The role of mirroring processes in social cognition (pp. 191-212). New York, NY: Humana Press, Springer Science.

Rochat, P., & Zahavi, D. (2011). The uncanny mirror: A re-framing of mirror self-experience. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 204-213. doi: 10.1016/j. concog.2010.06.007

Rochat, P., Passos-Ferreira, C., & Salem, P. (2009). Three levels of intersubjectivity in early development. In A. Carassa, F. Morganti, and G. Riva (Eds.), Enacting intersubjectivity: Paving the way for a dialogue between cognitive science, social cognition and neuroscience (pp. 173-190). Lugano: Università della Svizzera Italiana.

Rogoff, B. (1999): Cognitive development through social interaction: Vygotsky and Piaget. In: P. Murphy (Ed.), Learners, learning and assessment (pp. 69-82). London: SAGE Publishing.

Rommetveit, R. (1976). On the architecture of intersubjectivity. In L. H. Strickland, K. J. Gergen, & F. J. Aboud (Eds.), Social psychology in transition (pp. 163-175). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Rossano, F. (2013). Sequence organization and timing of bonobo mother-infant interactions. Interaction studies, 14, 160-189. doi: 10.1075/is.14.2.02ros

Rossmanith, N., Costall, A., Reichelt, A. F., López, B., & Reddy, V. (2014). Jointly structuring triadic spaces of meaning and action: Book sharing from 3 months on. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1390. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01390

Rohr, von, C. R., van Schaik, C. P., Kissling, A., & Burkart, J. M. (2015). Chimpanzees’ Bystander Reactions to Infanticide. Human Nature, 26, 143-160. doi: 10.1007/s12110-015-9228-5

Ruffman, T., Taumoepeau, M., & Perkins, C. (2012). Statistical learning as a basis for social understanding in children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 87-104. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02045.x

Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175-176. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.12.2.175

Schaffer, H. R. (1984). The child’s entry into a social world. Behavioural Development: A Series of Monographs. http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1985-17094-001, accessed: December 6, 2015.

Schiffrin, D. (1990). The principle of intersubjectivity in communication and conversation. Semiotica, 80 (1-2), 21-185. doi: 10.1515/semi.1990.80.1- 2.121

Schutz, A. (1962) Collected Papers, Vol. 1. Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

Searle, J. R. (1976). A classification of illocutionary acts. Language in Society, 5, 1-23. doi: 10.1017/S0047404500006837

Scaife, M., & Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature, 253, 265-266. doi: 10.1038/253265a0

Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction: A primer in conversation analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Sharrock, W. W. (1974). On owning knowledge. In R. Turner (Ed.), Ethnomethodology (pp. 45-53). Harmondsworth: Penguin.

Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 81-96. doi: 10.1111/j.1749- 6632.2009.04418.x

Slocombe, K. E., & Zuberbuehler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 17228-17233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706741104

Smith, C. B., Adamson, L.B., & Bakeman, R. (1988). Interactional predictors of early language. First Language, 8, 143-56. doi: 10.1177/01427237880 0802304

Sorjonen, M.-L., & Peräkylä, A. (2012). Introduction. In A. Peräkylä & M.- L. Sorjonen (Eds.), Emotion in interaction (pp. 3-15). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Stalnaker, R. (2002). Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25, 701-721. doi: 10.1023/A:1020867916902

Stern, D. N., Spieker, S., & MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology, 18(5), 727-735. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.727

Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Stevanovic, M. & Peräkylä, A. (2012). Deontic authority in interaction: The right to announce, propose and decide. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 45(3), 297-321. doi: 10.1080/08351813.2012.699260

Stevanovic, M. (2013). Constructing a proposal as a thought: A way to manage problems in the initiation of joint decision-making in Finnish workplace interaction. Pragmatics, 23(3), 519-544. doi: 10.1075/prag.23.3.07ste

Stevanovic, M., & Peräkylä, A. (2014). Three orders in the organization of human action: On the interface between knowledge, power, and emotion in interaction and social relations. Language in Society, 43, 185-207. doi: 10.1017/S0047404514000037

Stivers, T., Sidnell, J., & Bergen, C. (2018). Children’s responses to questions in peer interaction: A window into the ontogenesis of interactional competence. Journal of Pragmatics, 124, 14-30. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2017.11.013

Striano, T., & Rochat, P. (2000). Emergence of selective social referencing in infancy. Infancy, 1, 253-264. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0102_7

Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Tomasello, M., & Farrar M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454-63. doi: 10.2307/1130423

Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121-125. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x

Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231-55. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675-91. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129

Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation. Current Anthropology, 53, 673-692. doi: 10.1086/668207

Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind & Language, 18(2), 121-147. doi: 10.1111/1468-0017.00217

Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tomasello, M. (2014). A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginnings of human communication (pp. 99-136). London: Cambridge University Press.

Trevarthen, C. (1998). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In S. Bråten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 15-46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. J. (2001). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 3-48. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00701

Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183-229). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L.,Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infants’ response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interactions. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1-13. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)62273-1

Vaillant-Molina, M., & Bahrick, L. E. (2012). The role of intersensory redundancy in the emergence of social referencing in 5½ -month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1-9. doi: 10.1037/a0025263

Vaish, A., Missana, M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Three-year-old children intervene in third-party moral transgressions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 124-30. doi: 10.1348/026151010X532888

Venezia, M., Messinger, D.S., Thorp, D., & Mundy, P. (2004). The development of anticipatory smiling. Infancy, 6, 397-406. doi: 10.1207/s15327078 in0603_5

Vogel, A., de Koyer, I., Bellagamba, F., & Bell, H. (2002). The dialogical self in the first two years of life. Theory & Psychology, 12, 191-205. doi: 10.1177/0959354302012002629

von Scheve, C. (2012). The social calibration of emotion expression: An affective basis of micro-social order. Sociological Theory, 30(1), 1-14. doi: 10.1177/0735275112437163

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Wagner, H.L., & Smith, J. (1991). Facial expression in the presence of friends and strangers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 15, 201-214. doi: 10.1007/ BF00986922

Walden, T. A., & Ogan, T. A. (1988). The development of social referencing. Child Development, 59, 1230-1240. doi: 10.2307/1130486

Warneken, F., Gräfenhain, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young children’s understanding of joint intentions in collaborative activities. Developmental Science, 15, 54-61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01107.x

Wilkinson, R. (1999). Sequentiality as a problem and a resource for intersubjectivity in aphasic conversation: Analysis and implications for therapy. Aphasiology, 13(4), 327-343. doi: 10.1080/026870399402127

Zahavi, D., & Rochat, P. (2015). Empathy ≠ sharing: Perspectives from phenomenology and developmental psychology. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 543-553. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.05.008

Zeman, J., & Garber, J. (1996). Display rules for anger, sadness, and pain: It depends on who is watching. Child Development, 67, 957-973. doi: 10.1111/ j.1467-8624.1996.tb01776.x

Zlatev, J. (2008). The co-evolution of intersubjectivity and bodily mimesis. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 215-244). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

Journal Information


CiteScore 2017: 0.34

SCImago Journal Rank (SJR) 2017: 0.144
Source Normalized Impact per Paper (SNIP) 2017: 0.359

Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 0 0 0
Full Text Views 493 493 24
PDF Downloads 148 148 14